Acoustic communication in Australian fur seals

Abstract Communication is a fundamental process that allows animals to effectively transfer information between groups or individuals. Recognition plays an essential role in permitting animals to distinguish individuals based upon both communicatory and non-communicatory signals allowing animals to direct suitable behaviours towards them. Several modes of recognition exist and in colonial breeding animals which congregate in large numbers, acoustic signalling is thought to be the most effective as it suffers less from environmental degradation. Otariid seals (fur seals and sea lions) are generally colonial breeding species which congregate at high densities on offshore islands. In contrast to the other Arctocephaline species, the Australian fur seal, Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus, along with its conspecific, the Cape fur seal, A. p. pusillus, display many of the behavioural traits of sea lions. This may have important consequences in terms of its social structure and evolution. The acoustic communication of Australian fur seals was studied on Kanowna Island, Bass Strait, Australia. Analysing the acoustic structure of vocalisations and their use facilitates our understanding of the social function of calls in animal communication. The vocal repertoires of males, females, pups and yearlings were characterised and their behavioural context examined. Call structural variations in males were evident with changes in behavioural context, indicating parallel changes in the emotive state of sender. For a call to be used in vocal recognition it must display stereotypy within callers and variation between them. In Australian fur seal females and pups, individuals were found to have unique calls. Mutual mother-pup recognition has been suggested for otariids and this study supports the potential for this process to occur through the use of vocalisations. Call structural changes in pup vocalisations were also investigated over the progression of the year, from birth to weaning. Vocalisations produced by pups increased in duration, lowered in both the number of parts per call and the harmonic band containing the maximum frequency as they became older, suggesting calls are changing constantly as pups grow toward maturity. It has been suggested through descriptive reports, that the bark call produced by males is important to vocal recognition. The present study quantified this through the analysis of vocalisations produced by male Australian fur seals. Results support descriptive evidence suggesting that male barks can be used to discriminate callers. Traditional playback studies further confirmed that territorial male Australian fur seals respond significantly more to the calls of strangers than to those of neighbours, supporting male vocal recognition. This study modified call features of the bark to determine the importance to vocal recognition. The results indicate that the whole frequency spectrum was important to recognition. There was also an increase in response from males when they heard more bark units, indicating the importance of repetition by a caller. Recognition occurred when males heard between 25-75% of each bark unit, indicating that the whole duration of each bark unit is not necessary for recognition to occur. This may have particular advantages for communication in acoustically complex breeding environments, where parts of calls may be degraded by the environment. The present study examined the life history characteristics of otariids to determine the factors likely to influence and shape its vocal behaviour. Preliminary results indicate that female density, body size and the breeding environment all influence the vocal behaviour of otariids, while duration of lactation and the degree of polygyny do not appear to be influential. Understanding these interactions may help elucidate how vocal recognition and communication have evolved in different pinniped species. TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS STATEMENT OF ORGINALITY TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES LIST OF TABLES LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS LIST OF SCIENTIFIC NAMESGLOSSARY OF TERMS ABSTRACT CHAPTER 1. General Introduction 1.1 Communication and vocal recognition 1.2 Otariid pinnipeds 1.3 Australian fur seals 1.4 Overall aims and structure of the study CHAPTER 2. Characterisation of Australian fur seal vocalisations 2.1 Introduction 2.2 Materials and Methods 2.3 Results 2.3.1 Affiliative calls 2.3.2 Aggressive vocalisations 2.3.3 Dual function call 2.3.4 Behavioural context of the bark call 2.4 Discussion CHAPTER 3. Individual variation in the pup attraction call produced by female Australian fur seals during early lactation 3.1 Introduction 3.2 Materials and Methods 3.2.1 Study species 3.2.2 Data collection and acoustic analyses 3.2.3 Description of the Pup Attraction Call 3.2.4 Statistical analysis of the Pup Attraction Call 3.2.5 Peak frequency distribution in the Pup Attraction 3.3 Results 3.3.1 Description of the Pup Attraction Call 3.3.2 Inter-individual variation 3.3.3 Classification of variables 3.3.4 Peak frequency distribution in the Pup Attraction Call 3.4 Discussion CHAPTER 4. Changes in call structure of Australian fur seal pups throughout the maternal dependency period 4.1 Introduction 4.2 Materials and Methods 4.2.1 Data collection and acoustic analyses 4.2.2 Statistical analysis of the Female Attraction Call 4.2.3 Call structure changes with age 4.3 Results 4.3.1 Potential for Individual Coding 4.3.2 Discriminant Function Analysis 4.3.3 Classification And Regression Tree analysis 4.3.4 Call structure changes with age 4.4 Discussion CHAPTER 5. Species-specific characteristics and individual variation of the bark call produced by male Australian fur seals, Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus 5.1 Introduction 5.2 Materials and Methods 5.2.1 Data Collection 5.2.2 Definitions 5.2.3 Acoustic Analysis 5.2.4 Characterisation of the bark call 5.2.5 Statistical analyses 5.3 Results 5.3.1 Characterisation of the bark call 5.3.2 Intra- versus inter- individual variation 5.3.3 Inter-individual variation 5.4 Discussion CHAPTER 6. Who goes there? The dear-enemy effect in male Australian fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus 6.1 Introduction 6.2 Materials and Methods 6.2.1 Study site and recording methods 6.2.2 Design of playback experiments 6.2.3 Criteria of Response 6.3 Results 6.4 Discussion CHAPTER 7. General Discussion 7.1 Functionality of calls 7.2 Factors influencing the acoustic behaviour of Otariids 7.3 Are Australian fur seals vocalisations more like sea lions 7.3 Future research LIST OF REFERENCES Acoustic communication in Australian fur seals(Tạm dịch:Acoustic truyền thông trong hải cẩu Úc)

pdf134 trang | Chia sẻ: maiphuongtl | Lượt xem: 2314 | Lượt tải: 0download
Bạn đang xem trước 20 trang tài liệu Acoustic communication in Australian fur seals, để xem tài liệu hoàn chỉnh bạn click vào nút DOWNLOAD ở trên
ce of a neighbour-stranger vocal discrimination in male Australian fur seals. It also demonstrated that there was an increase in response from males when they heard more bark units from stranger seals, indicating that the number of units was important for eliciting a response from males. Furthermore, altering the inter-unit spaces of neighbouring male calls did not affect the response of male subjects, which was surprising as this result is not consistent with other studies and further experimental playbacks are suggested to examine this further. Playback experiments also indicated that the whole frequency spectrums of calls are important to recognition. Finally, investigations reveal that males may only need to hear between 25 - 75% of each bark unit for recognition to occur. Further research examining the importance of behavioural posturing to the recognition process and examining the location of callers on territories would provide useful insights into the importance of these factors to vocal recognition in male Australian fur seals. 122 CHAPTER 7 GENERAL DISCUSSION The Australian fur seal is a highly vocal marine mammal, breeding in dense social groups on only ten islands in the Bass Strait. As with other pinnipeds, vocalisations in this species are pivotal to reproductive exchanges and are important on a number of different levels in social organisation, including mother- offspring interactions and male territorial defence. However, while the importance of understanding the acoustic behaviour of pinnipeds is clear, this behaviour in Australian fur seals has received very little attention in the literature, with only one previous study providing descriptive evidence of vocalisations (Stirling and Warneke 1971). The present study investigated the acoustic behaviour of Australian fur seals by examining wild seals at the remote breeding colony on Kanowna Island, with the aim of describing the physical structure and behavioural context of the animals’ calls. The study examined the individuality of vocalisations and the acoustic characteristics that are important in separating callers. These identified characteristics may be used in vocal recognition. It also examined the call structure of pup vocalisations and how these change through the maternal dependency period, then tested vocal recognition in territorial males thus the present study expands the understanding of the acoustic behaviour and vocal recognition abilities of this species and provides information of broader relevance to other colonial breeding species. 7.1 Functionality of vocalisations Species recognition is important as it allows individuals to recognise other animals as conspecifics, thereby deterring them from interbreeding. By conducting baseline studies on the vocalisations produced by Australian fur seals, the study was able to compare acoustic characteristics between species. Results indicate the PAC, FAC and bark that are produced by Australian fur seal 123 females, pups and males, respectively, are structurally similar in their gross morphology and function to the vocalisations of other otariid species (Fernández- Juricic et al. 1999; Phillips and Stirling 2000; Page et al. 2002 a and b). However, the species are clearly distinguishable in their acoustic characteristics. In particular, the PAC (females), FAC (pups) and bark (males) produced by Australian fur seals, have a lower fundamental frequency (Phillips and Stirling 2000; Page et al. 2002a; Roux 1986). In male Australian fur seals, the fundamental frequency is much lower than in other fur seals, being at least 250 Hz lower than the barks of the subantarctic fur seals (Roux 1986). In general, a direct relationship exists between the size of an animal and the frequency it can produce, with larger animals capable of producing lower frequency calls (Morton 1977). These species differences may function in keeping species discrete, this aspect being particularly important with respect to sympatrically occurring species, such as Australian and New Zealand fur seals. A number of vocalisations are employed by both males and females in territorial defence. These are aggressive vocalisations that are structurally low in frequency and pulsed. In male Australian fur seals, the calls used in the defence of territories include the bark and the guttural threat, whereas the full threat call and growl that are reported in other fur seals is missing in this species. In female Australian fur seals, the aggressive vocalisations include the bark, guttural threat and growl. Aggressive vocalisations in females have received little attention in the literature, with the main focus being on mother-offspring vocalisations. However, in resource defence polygyny, evidence suggests that females defend resources within the male’s territory, and this in turn may limit the number of mates available to the territorial male and reduce his mating success (Carey 1992). On the basis of descriptive evidence it has been suggested that the bark produced by male Australian fur seals is important to vocal recognition (Stirling and Warneke 1971). In the dear-enemy effect, territorial individuals which compete to defend a resource area typically respond more to strangers (i.e. unfamiliar individuals) than to neighbouring animals (Fisher 1954). This variation 124 in response may be based on the perceived level of threat posed by the different individuals (Temeles 1994). In the present study, the call structure of the male bark was analysed to determine if the vocalisations of Australian fur seals fit this theory. Using traditional analytical techniques only, evidence was sought to establish that bark calls were individually distinct. The more novel technique (i.e. CART) was not deemed necessary as most call features could be incorporated into the traditional techniques (i.e. PIC and DFA). Both frequency and temporal parameters were reported as necessary in separating individual callers. The neighbour-stranger recognition system in males was tested in Australian fur seals using playback experiments and the results indicate that territorial males respond more to the calls of strangers than to the calls of their neighbours, supporting neighbour-stranger recognition in this species. Acoustic modifications of the bark call parameters were used to assess their importance to vocal recognition. The whole frequency spectrum was found to be important to recognition. Furthermore, recognition occurred when males heard between 25- 75% of each bark unit from seals indicating that the whole duration of each bark unit is not necessary for recognition to occur. This may have particular advantages for communication in acoustically complex breeding areas, where calls may be degraded by the environment. Other acoustic manipulations where the inter-unit spaces were increased and decreased by 25%, did not elicit changes in the response of males. This result was surprising as the outcome from the individual variation study indicated that the inter-unit duration would be important. A substantial proportion of male barks, including those associated with nuzzling and mating, were directed to females during the breeding season. This suggests that these calls are important in inter-sexual relations and mate attraction. In South American sea lions, there is a high degree of association between male vocal behaviour and the factors that influence male mating success, with vocal rates increasing as males monopolize larger numbers of females (Fernández-Juricic et al. 2001). 125 Vocalisations in infant animals are typically high frequency and tonal, vocal characteristics that are adapted for eliciting parental care (Morton 1977). To date, descriptive and qualitative evidence in otariids suggests that calls used by mothers and pups during the reunion process contain unique properties that enable vocal recognition (Insley et al. 2003b). In Australian fur seals, similar to other otariids, mothers and pups experience repeated separations and reunions over an extended lactation period. A mechanism for mutual recognition is vital for both mother and pup as both benefit from a successful reunion. This reunion process may be facilitated through a multi-modal sensory system using a combination of vocal, olfactory and spatial cues. In a crowded breeding colony acoustic communication is considered more efficient, as it is less constrained by environmental factors. The present study investigated call individuality in Australian fur seal mother-pup vocalisations. Results indicated that the PAC (females) and FAC (pups) contain enough information to permit the discrimination of individual mothers and offspring. Using a combination of traditional and non-traditional techniques, several acoustic parameters were suggested to be important for recognition. Investigations into the call structure changes of newborn pups to 11 months of age indicate that calls increase in duration, lower in both the number of parts per call and the harmonic band containing the maximum frequency. These call modifications may be related to the growth and weight changes (Arnould and Hindell 2002), as well as lengthening of the vocal cords and increases in lung capacity, as reported in other vertebrate species (Snowdown and Elowson 1992). This study, together with others, support the hypothesis that long term maternal recognition of offspring may be facilitated through mothers learning new versions of pup vocalisations as these modify throughout postnatal development and maturation (Charrier et al. 2003c). In general the individuality studies of males, females and pups produced some parallels in the call parameters that were important in separating callers. The fundamental frequency and duration were valuable in separating callers in all categories (i.e. females, pups and males). In addition, the maximum peak 126 frequency was essential in separating pup and male callers, and the inter-unit duration was important in separating male callers only. The PAC (females), FAC (pups) and bark (males) of Australian fur seals are all moderately stereotyped, when compared to other fur seal studies. This may imply that other sensory signals such as vision and olfaction may be used in the recognition process by this species. 7.2 Factors influencing the vocal behaviour of Otariids The present study examined characteristics that relate to the breeding biology of otariids that are likely to influence their acoustic behaviour. There are large gaps in the literature in these areas amongst the otariid species and this lack of knowledge needs to be addressed before the results of the present study can be analysed in detail. There are also differences in sample size, and differences in the replicates per individual which may account for some of the variances in results between species. Nevertheless, comparisons were made using results from the literature and some preliminary trends are reported. Species breeding at low densities have more calls in their repertoire as opposed to species breeding in higher female densities, which have in general, lower repertoire sizes (Table 7.1). This pattern is also known to occur in birds, where smaller repertoire sizes are used by males that have access to more females (Catchpole 1980). Vocalisations of polygynous species have evolved primarily through intra-sexual selection where calls are simpler, shorter and stereotyped in structure used in male-male interactions, while vocalisations used in monogamous species, have evolved for use in sexual attraction, where songs are more elaborate, long and complex (Catchpole 1980). Although all otariids are polygynous, this argument may be applied in part to these seals. The selective pressures on species where female congregations on breeding areas are dense may be different to those breeding on areas where there are fewer females. Where females are more densely spaced, calls in males may function primarily in male-male interactions and may need to be simple and repetitive. On the other hand males holding territories in which females are more widely 127 dispersed may need to defend their territories, but may also attract females to some degree. The requirement to attract females may account for the larger repertoire sizes for these species. There is a general trend toward greater vocal repertoire in those species breeding on boulder and jumbled rock areas as opposed to those breeding on more open areas (some exceptions e.g., Antarctic fur seal). Roux and Jouventin (1987) suggested that species inhabiting boulder type areas may need to use more call types for communication as other sensory modes, such as vision in relation to behavioural displays, may be constrained by the physical environment. In contrast, species such Australian fur seals that breed in more open areas, might be able to utilise both calls in combination with other sensory modalities for communication, thereby reducing the need for larger call repertoires. It is suggested that this feature, in association with others, may influence the vocal behaviour of otariids. The degree of call stereotypy is fairly high in all species investigated, implying vocal recognition can be used by all species. In females the greatest difference in call stereotypy was between South American fur seals (70%) and South American sea lions (90%). This may be related to female density where the need to be more stereotyped, (i.e. more recognisable) to pups may be greater in more crowded areas, such as those in South American sea lions. Similarly, in pup vocalisations, the call stereotypy is lowest in Antarctic and South American fur seals and highest in South American sea lions. This may be a reflection of similar selective pressures for this behaviour which has led to more stereotyped calls, where the acoustic features in South American sea lion pup calls are more distinctive, allowing mothers to recognise their offspring. There is also a notable difference in the fundamental frequency amongst otariid species in all categories compared (i.e. males, females and pups), being lower in Australian fur seals compared with other fur seal species (Table 7.1). This difference may be related to body size where larger body size of Australian fur seals enables them to produce lower frequency vocalisations (Morton 1977). This aspect was also reported as an influential factor in shaping the acoustic 128 behaviour of male phocids (Rogers 2003). The degree of polygyny and length of lactation (Rogers et al. 2003) did not appear to be influence the acoustic behaviour of both otariids and phocids. In summary female density, body size and breeding environment all appear to influence the vocal behaviour of otariids, while duration of lactation and degree of polygyny do not appear to be influential. 1 2 9 T a b le 7 .1 L if e h is to ry c h a ra c te ri s ti c s a n d c a ll fe a tu re s o f O ta ri id s . S P E C IE S D u ra ti o n o f la c ta ti o n (m o n th s ) 1 ,2 D e g re e o f p o ly g y n y 3 F e m a le d e n s it y (f /m 2 ) 4 S o c ia l o rg a n iz a ti o n 3 H a b it a t 4 , 5 , 6 M a le w e ig h t (k g ) 7 F e m a le w e ig h t (k g ) 7 N o rt h e rn f u r s e a ls , C a llo rh in u s U rs in u s 3 -4 E x tr e m e p o ly g y n y 0 .2 -0 .6 E x tr e m e ly l a rg e g ro u p s ; fe m a le s d e n s e ly s p a c e d Is la n d , b ro k e n b a s a lt ; b o u ld e r b e a c h e s 2 2 7 .0 4 4 .8 S u b a n ta rc ti c f u r s e a ls , A . tr o p ic a lis 1 0 -1 1 M o d e ra te t o e x tr e m e p o ly g y n y 0 .1 S m a ll to l a rg e g ro u p s ; fe m a le s w e ll s p a c e d Is la n d ; ju m b le d r o c k y c o a s tl in e 1 5 2 .5 5 0 .0 N e w Z e a la n d f u r s e a ls , A . fo rs te ri 9 -1 2 P o ly g y n y 0 .1 S m a ll to l a rg e g ro u p s ; fe m a le s w e ll s p a c e d Is la n d ; j u m b le d r o c k y c o a s tl in e 1 6 4 .4 5 5 .0 A n ta rc ti c f u r s e a l, A . g a z e lle 4 M o d e ra te t o e x tr e m e p o ly g y n y 0 .4 -1 .1 S m a ll to l a rg e g ro u p s ; fe m a le s p a c in g v a ri a b le Is la n d , a n d o p e n b e a c h e s 1 5 5 .0 3 8 .2 S o u th A m e ri c a n f u r s e a ls , A . A u s tr a lis 7 -3 6 P o ly g y n y 0 .5 -1 .0 S m a ll g ro u p s ; fe m a le s w e ll s p a c e d Is la n d a n d c o a s ta l; ro c k s h e lv e s 1 5 9 .0 4 8 .5 G a la p a g o s f u r s e a l, A . G a la p a g o e n s is 2 4 P o ly g y n y 0 .0 4 S m a ll g ro u p s ; fe m a le s w e ll s p a c e d Is la n d ; ro c k s h e lv e s ; b o u ld e r b e a c h e s 6 4 .5 2 7 .4 S o u th A fr ic a n f u r s e a l, A . p u s ill u s . p u s ill u s 6 -1 2 M o d e ra te t o e x tr e m e p o ly g y n y 1 .4 -1 .9 M o d e ra te -s iz e d t o e x tr e m e ly l a rg e g ro u p s ; fe m a le s d e n s e ly s p a c e d Is la n d a n d c o a s ta l, ro c k s h e lv e s 2 7 8 .0 7 1 .0 A u s tr a lia n f u r s e a ls , A . p . D o ri fe ru s 1 1 -1 2 M o d e ra te t o e x tr e m e p o ly g y n y 0 .2 S m a ll to l a rg e g ro u p s ; fe m a le s d e n s e ly s p a c e d Is la n d a n d c o a s ta l, ro c k s h e lv e s , o p e n te rr a in 3 0 7 .0 0 8 4 .0 C a lif o rn ia n s e a l io n , Z a lo p h u s c a lif o rn ia n u s 4 -8 (u p t o 1 2 ) M o d e ra te t o e x tr e m e p o ly g y n y 0 .1 -0 .2 M o d e ra te -s iz e d t o l a rg e g ro u p s ; fe m a le s d e n s e ly s p a c e d Is la n d ; s a n d b e a c h e s , ro c k s h e lv e s 2 8 9 .0 8 6 .0 S o u th A m e ri c a n s e a l io n s , O ta ri a f la v e s c e n s 5 -1 2 M o d e ra te p o ly g y n y - M o d e ra te -s iz e d t o l a rg e g ro u p s ; fe m a le s d e n s e ly s p a c e d Is la n d a n d c o a s ta l; ro c k s h e lv e s , s h in g le b e a c h e s 3 0 0 .0 1 4 4 .0 1 3 0 1 = A tk in s o n 1 9 9 7 ; 2 = B o w e n 1 9 9 1 ; 3 = R ie d m a n 1 9 9 0 ; 4 = B o n e s s = 1 9 9 1 ; 5 = G o ld s w o rt h y e t a l. 1 9 9 9 ; 6 = P a g e e t a l. 2 0 0 2 (a ); 7 = L in d e n fo rs e t a l 2 0 0 2 ; 8 = P h ill ip s a n d S ti rl in g 2 0 0 1 ; 9 = S ti rl in g a n d W a rn e k e 1 9 7 1 ; 1 0 = T ri p o v ic h e t a l. 2 0 0 5 ; 1 1 = F e rn a n d e z -J u ri c ic e t a l. , 1 9 9 9 ; 1 2 = P e te rs o n a n d B a rt h o lo m e w 1 9 6 9 ; 1 3 = R o u x 1 9 8 6 ; 1 4 = T ri p o v ic h e t a l. 2 0 0 6 , 1 5 = I n s le y 1 9 9 2 ; 1 6 = c u rr e n t s tu d y . S P E C IE S N o o f c a lls in m a le re p e rt o ir e 8 , 9 , 1 0 , 1 1 , 1 2 % c o rr e c t D F A m a le b a rk 1 0 , 1 1 , 1 3 M a le F o 1 0 , 1 3 % c o rr e c t D F A fe m a le P A C 6 , 8 , 1 1 , 1 4 , 1 5 F e m a le F o 6 , 8 , 1 4 % c o rr e c t D F A p u p F A C 6 , 8 , 1 1 , 1 5 , 1 6 P u p F o 6 , 8 , 1 6 N o rt h e rn f u r s e a ls , C a llo rh in u s U rs in u s ? - - 8 2 - 7 9 x S u b a n ta rc ti c f u r s e a ls , A . tr o p ic a lis 3 -7 x 3 9 1 ( 5 5 ) 8 4 5 0 6 ( 1 4 ) 8 3 5 2 0 ( 1 9 ) N e w Z e a la n d f u r s e a ls , A . fo rs te ri 5 -7 x x 8 8 5 3 0 ( 6 1 ) 7 9 7 2 9 ( 6 3 ) A n ta rc ti c f u r s e a l, A . g a z e lle 5 x X 7 4 7 6 0 ( 1 4 ) 5 2 6 6 6 ( 1 2 ) S o u th A m e ri c a n f u r s e a ls , A . A u s tr a lis 7 x x 7 0 9 0 5 ( 1 1 .4 ) 6 0 1 0 3 0 (1 8 .5 ) G a la p a g o s f u r s e a l, A . G a la p a g o e n s is 7 x x x x x x S o u th A fr ic a n f u r s e a l, A . p u s ill u s . p u s ill u s 3 -7 x x x x x x A u s tr a lia n f u r s e a ls , A . p . D o ri fe ru s 3 8 3 1 4 0 .3 ( 2 4 .1 ) 7 6 2 6 2 ( 3 4 .6 ) 7 5 3 4 2 ( 6 2 ) C a lif o rn ia n s e a l io n , Z a lo p h u s c a lif o rn ia n u s 2 x x x x x x S o u th A m e ri c a n s e a l io n s , O ta ri a f la v e s c e n s 4 7 6 x 9 0 x 9 0 x 131 7.3 Are Australian fur seals vocalisations more like sea lions? Previously, it has been suggested that the behaviour and vocalisations of Australian fur seals resemble more of sea lions rather than those of fur seals (Warneke and Shaughnessy 1985). Throughout the study it was apparent that differences existed in the vocal characteristics between Australian fur seals and other fur seal species but due to the lack of sufficient data, detailed comparisons were not possible. However what was evident was that the fundamental frequency characteristic of Australian fur seal vocalisations was more similar to sea lions than to those of fur seals. The opinion of the author is that both the larger body size and breeding environment may influence and shape the vocalisations of seals. Firstly, Australian fur seals are the largest of all fur seals (Table 7.1) and are comparative in size to other sea lions, and in general, larger animals are capable of producing lower frequency calls (Morton 1977) which may account for the lower frequency (particularly in barks produced by males) reported for Australian fur seals and sea lions compared with other fur seals. And secondly, the differences in the vocalisations may be related to the variation in breeding environments. In general, Australian fur seals and sea lions (from available data to date) produce lower frequency bark calls and breed in more open areas, while fur seals produce higher frequency barks and breed on more jumbled rocky areas (Table 7.1). The potential for scattering of vocalisations is greater in jumbled rocky areas and so having higher frequency calls that are more directional would be advantageous in those environments, which may explain for the differences in frequencies between the Australian fur seals and sea lions compared with all other fur seals (Wiley and Richards 1978). 7.4 Future research Acoustic signals play a very important role in the breeding success of a wide variety of species. The current study described the calls produced by males, females, yearlings and pups throughout the breeding season, providing valuable information on the vocal behaviour of a species that has not been studied in great detail. This information provided by this study allows the opportunity for researchers to compare this species with other pinnipeds and the results may elucidate general evolutionary patterns. 132 Future research could extend the playback studies to investigate the recognition abilities of mothers and offspring. Artificial modification of the calls would be advised in order to determine the call features that are required for the individual identification process. The choice of variables for modification could be based on the results of the individual call variability studies, which highlight the call parameters most likely to be important to the recognition process. Behavioural development studies involve examination of the changes that occur as the young grows and matures (Martin and Bateson 1993). The present study investigated changes in pup vocalisations utilising a cross- sectional sampling approach. An alternative method of analysis known as longitudinal analysis, involves sampling the same individuals through time. It would be ideal if seal pups could be sampled using longitudinal in addition to cross-sectional techniques and results compared. In the present study the recognition abilities of male Australian fur seals was investigated. Future experiments testing a male’s ability to recognise individual neighbours would prove interesting as it can reveal whether male vocal recognition is based on males recognising individuals or recognising a group of animals as ether familiar or unfamiliar. Furthermore while the present study reports valuable information on a colonial male seal species, it would be interesting to conduct playback studies on male seals that utilise other breeding strategies such as those that of solitary species (e.g., leopard seals, Hydrurga leptonyx) or those having harems (e.g., southern elephant seals, Mirounga leonina). This may provide further insights into the evolutionary patterns or environmental constraints affecting social communication in pinnipeds. Vocal communication and recognition between mothers and offspring and between males may involve other features (e.g. amplitude modulation, sound pressure levels, and others) not measured in the present study. These call features may improve the percent correct classification scores. Other features such vision, spatial orientation and smell may also play a role in the recognition process and investigations on these factors could help reveal 133 important information on the recognition process in seals and in understanding the reproductive success in fur seals. Lastly, vocalisations emitted by Australian fur seals are not produced in isolation. It appears that different call types are used in combination during certain behavioural contexts. Other studies on primates have indicated that these call combinations are not produced randomly and that the order of the combinations may have meaning (Crockford and Boesch 2005). This study has provided the necessary baseline descriptions of single call units, which can then be utilised to investigate the importance of call combinations in the communicative process. Sexual reproduction creates a social environment of conflict and competition among individuals as each attempt to maximise its genetic contribution to subsequent generations (Alcock 1993). Vocalisations are a major component in the breeding communication of Australian fur seals. Consequently, investigations made by this study broaden our understanding on the acoustic behaviour of Australian fur seals and the influences shaping vocalisations, all of which may ultimately impact the breeding success of an individual. 134 LIST OF REFERENCES Alcock, J., 1993. Animal Behaviour: an evolutionary approach. Fifth Edition. Sinauer Associates Inc. Publishers, Sunderland, Massachusetts, USA. Arnould, J.P.Y. and Duck, C.D. 1997. The costs and benefits of territorial tenure, and factors affecting mating success, in male Antarctic fur seals. Journal of Zoology London 241, 649-664. Arnould, J.P.Y. and Hindell, M. 2002. Milk consumption, body rates of Australian fur seal pups. Journal of Zoology London 256, 351-356 Arnould, J.P.Y. and Hindell, M.A. 2001. Dive behavior, foraging locations, and maternal attendance patterns of Australian fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus). Canadian Journal of Zoology 79, 35-48. Atkinson, S. 1997. Reproductive biology of seals. Reviews of Reproduction 2, 175–194 Balcolme, J. P. and McCracken, G.F. 1992. Vocal recognition in Mexican free- tailed bats: do pups recognize mothers? Animal Behaviour 43, 79-87. Bartholomew, G. A. 1970. A model for the evolution of pinniped polygyny. Evolution 24, 546-559. Bee, M. A. 2003. A test of the ‘dear-enemy effect’ in the strawberry dart-poison frog (Dendrobates pumillio). Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 54, 601-610. Bee, M.A., and Gerhardt, H.C. 2001a. Neighbour-stranger discrimination by territorial male bullfrogs (Rana catesbeiana): I. Acoustic basis. Animal Behaviour 62, 1129-1140 Bee, M.A., and Gerhardt, H.C. 2001b. Neighbour-stranger discrimination by territorial male bullfrogs (Rana catesbeiana): II. Perceptual basis. Animal Behaviour 62, 1141-1150. Bee, M.A., Kozich, C.E., Blackwell, K.J. and Gerhardt, H.C. 2001. Individual variation in advertisement calls of territorial male green tree frogs, Rana clamitans: Implications for individual discrimination. Ethology 107, 65-84. 135 Bininda-Emonds, O.R.P., Gittleman, J.L., and Purvis, A. 1999. Building large trees by combining phylogenetic information: a complete phylogeny of the extant Carnivora (Mammmalia). Biological Review 74, 143-175. Boness, D.J. 1991. Determinants of mating systems in the Otariidae (Pinnipedia). In: Behaviour of Pinnipeds . (D. Renouf ed.). Chapman and Hall, London. Bowen, W.D. 1991. Behavioral ecology of pinniped neonates. In: The Behavior of Pinnipeds. (D. Renouf, D. ed.). Chapman and Hall, London. Bradbury, J.W. and Vehrencamp, S.L. 1998. Principles of animal communication. Sinauer associates, Inc. Sunderland, M.A., U.S.A. Brunner, S. 2004. Fur seals and sea lions (Otariidae): identification of species and taxonomic review. Species and Biodiversity 1 (3): 339-439. Carey, P.W. 1992. Agonistic behaviour in female New Zealand fur seals, Arctocephalus forsteri. Ethology 92, 70-90. Castellano, S., and Giacoma, C. 1998. Stabilizing and directional female choice for male calls in the European green toad. Animal Behaviour 56, 275-287. Catchpole, C.K. 1980. Sexual selection and the evolution of complex songs among European warblers of the genus Acrocephalus. Behaviour 74,149– 166 Caudron, A.K., Kondakov, A.A. and Siryanov, S.V. 1998. Acoustic structure and individual variation of grey seal (Halichoerus grypus) pup calls. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 78, 651-658. Charrier, I. and Harcourt, R.G. 2006. Individual vocal identity in mother and pup Australian sea lions (Neophoca cinerea). Journal of Mammalogy 87, 929- 938. Charrier, I., Mathevon, N. and Jouventin, P. 2003a. Individuality in the voice of fur seal females: An analysis study of the pup attraction call in Arctocephalus tropicalis. Marine Mammal Science 19,161-172. Charrier, I., Mathevon, N., and Jouventin,P. 2003b. Vocal signature of mothers by fur seal pups. Animal Behaviour 65, 543-550. Charrier, I., Mathevon, N., and Jouventin, P. 2003c. Fur seal mother memorizes growing pup’s voice steps: Adaptation to the long-term recognition or evolutionary by-product? Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 80, 305-312. 136 Charrier, I., Mathevon, N. and Jouventin, P. 2002. How does a fur seal mother recognize the voice of her pup? An experimental study of Arctocephalus tropicalis. Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 603-612. Charrier, I., Mathevon, N., and Jouventin, P. 2001a. Mother’s voice recognition by seal pups. Newborns need to learn their mother’s call before she can take off on a fishing trip. Nature 412, 873. Charrier, I., Mathevon, N., Jouventin, P., and Aubin, T. 2001b. Acoustic communication in a black-headed gull colony: How do chicks identify their parents? Ethology 107, 961-974. Collins, K.T., Rogers, T.L., Terhune, J.M., McGreevy, P.D., Wheatley, K.E. and Harcourt, R.G. 2005. Individual variation of in-air female ‘pup contact’ calls in Weddell seals, Leptonychotes weddelli. Behaviour 142, 167-189. Conner, D.A. 1985. The function of the pika short call in individual recognition. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 67, 131–143. Crockford, C. and Boesch, C. 2005. Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 142, 397-421. De’ath, G., and Fabricius, K. 2000. Classification and regression tree: a powerful yet simple technique for ecological data analysis. Ecology 81, 3178-3192. Deméré T. A., Berta A., Adam P.J. 2003. Pinnipedimorph evolutionary biogeography. Bulletin of the American Museum of Natural History 279: 33–76. Dobson, A.J. 2001. An introduction to generalized linear models. Second edition. Chapman and Hall, London. Espmark, Y. 1971. Individual recognition by voice in reindeer mother-young relationship, field observations and playback experiments. Behaviour 40, 295-301. Evans, W.E. and Bastian, J. 1969. Marine mammal communication: social and ecological factors. In: The Biology of Marine Mammals. (H.T. Andersen ed.). Academic Press, New York. p. 425-475. Falls, J.B. 1982. Individual recognition by sounds in birds. In: Acoustic communication in birds, Vol. I. (D.E. Kroodsma and E.H. Miller eds). Academic Press, Inc., New York. 137 Fernández-Juricic, E., Campagna, C., Enriquez, V., and Ortiz, C.L. 2001. Vocal rates and social context in male South American sea lions. Marine Mammal Science 17, 387-396. Fernández-Juricic, E., Campagna, C., Enriquez, V., and Ortiz, C.L. 1999. Vocal communication and individual variation in breeding South American sea lions. Behaviour 136, 495-517. Fichtel, C., Hammerschmidt, K. and Jürgens, U. 2001. On the vocal expression of emotion. A multi-parametric analysis of different states of aversion in the squirrel monkey. Behaviour 138, 97-116. Fisher, J. 1954. Evolution and bird sociality. In: Evolution as a process. (J. Huxley, A.C. Hardy and E.B. Ford. eds.). George Allen and Unwin Ltd, London. Frommolt, K., Goltsman, M.E., and MacDonald, D.W. 2003. Barking foxes, Alopex lagopus: field experiments in individual recognition in a territorial mammal. Animal Behaviour 65, 509-518. Gentry, R.L. and Kooyman, G.L. 1986. Fur seals: Maternal strategies on land and at sea. (R.L. Gentry and G.L. Kooyman eds.). Princeton University Press, New Jersey. Goldsworthy, S.D., Boness, D.J., and Fleischer, R.C. 1999. Mate choice among sympatric fur seals: female preference for conphenotypic males. Behaviorual Ecology and Sociobiology 99, 253-267. Gubernick, D.J. 1981. Parent and infant attachment in mammals. In: Parental care in mammals. (D.J. Gubernick and P.H. Klopfer eds.). Plenum Press, New York. Hammerschimdt, K., Freudenstein, T., and Jürgens, U. 2001. Vocal development in squirrel monkeys. Behaviour 138, 1179-1204. Hanggi, E. B. 1992. The importance of vocal cues in mother–pup recognition in a California sea lion. Marine Mammal Science 8, 430–432. Harrington, F.H. 1989. Chorus howling by wolves: acoustic structure, pack size and the Beau Geste effect. Bioacoustics 2, 117-136. Honda-Sumi, E. 2005. Difference in calling song of three field crickets of the genus Teleogryllus: the role of premating isolation. Animal Behaviour 69, 881-889. 138 Hopp, S.L. and Morton, E.S. Sound Playback Studies. In: Animal acoustic communication. (S.L. Hopp, Owren, M.J., and Evans, C.S. eds.). Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. New York. p. 323-352. Hume, F., Arnould, J., Kirkwood, R. and Davis, P. 2001. Extended maternal dependence by juvenile Australian fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus). Australian Mammalogy 23, 67-70. Illmann, G., Schrader, L., Spinka, M., Sustr, P. 2002. Acoustical mother- recognition in pigs (Sus scrofa domestica). Behaviour 139, 487-505. Insley, S.J., Paredes, R. and Jones, I.L. 2003a. Sex differences in razorbill Alca torda parent-offspring vocal recognition. The Journal of Experimental Biology 206, 25-31. Insley, S.J., Phillips, A.V., and Charrier, I. 2003b. A review of social recognition in pinnipeds. Aquatic Mammals 29, 181-201. Insley, S.J. 2001. Mother-Offspring vocal recognition in northern fur seals is mutual but asymmetrical. Animal Behaviour 61, 129-137. Insley, S.J. 1992. Mother-offspring separation and acoustic stereotypy: A comparison of call morphology in two species of pinnipeds. Behaviour 120, 103-122. Job, D.A., Boness, D.J. and Francis, J.M. 1995. Individual variation in nursing voclalizations of Hawaiian monk seals, Monachus schauinslandi (Phocidae, Pinnipedia), and lack of maternal recognition. Canadian Journal of Zoology 73, 975- 983. Jouventin, P., Aubin, T., and Lengagne, T. 1999. Finding a parent in a king penguin colony: the acoustic system of individual recognition. Animal Behaviour 57, 1175-1183. Kirkwood, R., Gales, R., Terauds, A., Arnould, J.P.Y., Pemberton, D., Shaughnessy, P.D., Mitchell, A.T. and Gibbens, J. 2005. Pup production and population trends of the Australian fur seal (Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus). Marine Mammal Science 21, 260-282. Klecka, W.R. 1980. Discriminant analysis. Sage Publications, Beverly Hills, California. Lenhardt, M.L. 1977. Vocal contour cues in maternal recognition of goat kids. Applied Animal Ethology 3, 211-219. 139 Lindenfors, P., Tullberg, and Biuw, M. 2002. Phylogenetic analyses of sexual selection and sexual dimorphism in pinnipeds. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 52, 188-193. Lovell S.F., and Lein M.R. 2005. Individual recognition of neighbors by song in a suboscine bird, the alder flycatcher Empidonax alnorum. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 57, 623-630 Martin, P. and Bateson, P. 1986. Measuring behaviour. Second edition. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge. Mathevon, N. 1996. Individuality of contact calls in the Greater Flamingo Phoenicopterus ruber and the problem of background noise in a colony. Ibis 139, 513-517. McComb, K. 1987 Roaring by red deer stags advances that date of oestrus in hinds. Nature 330, 648-649. McComb, K., Moss, C., Sayialel, S. and Baker, L. 2000. Unusually extensive networks of vocal recognition in African Elephants. Animal Behaviour 59, 1103-1109. McCowan, B., Reiss,D., and Gubbins,C. 1998. Social familiarity influences whistle acoustic structure in adult female bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncates). Aquatic Mammals 21, 27-40. McCulloch, S., Pomeroy, P.P., and Slater, P.J.B. 1999. Individually distinctive pup vocalisations fail to prevent allo-suckling in grey seals. Canadian Journal of Zoology 77, 716-723. McElligott, A.G., and Hayden, T.J. 1999. Context-related vocalization rates of fallow bucks, Dama dama. Animal Behaviour 58, 1095-1104. Miller, E.H. 1991. Communication in pinnipeds, with special reference to non- acoustic signalling. In: Behaviour of Pinnipeds (D. Renouf ed.). Chapman and Hall, London. p. 128-235. Miller, E.H. 1982. Character and variance shift in acoustic signals of birds. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds. Volume 1. (D.E. Kroodsma, E.H. Miller and H. Ouellet, eds.) p. 253-295. Academic Press, New York, U.S.A. Møller, A.P. 1988. Spatial and temporal distribution of song in the yellowhammer Emberiza citronella. Ethology 78, 321-331. 140 Morton, E.S. 1977. On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. The American Naturalist 111, 855-869. Page, B., Goldsworthy, S. D. & Hindell, M. A. 2001. Vocal traits of hybrid fur seals: intermediate to their parental species. Animal Behaviour 61, 959–967. Page, B., Goldsworthy, S.D.and Hindell, M.A. 2002a Individual vocal traits of mother and pup fur seals. Bioacoustics 13, 121-143. Page, B., Goldsworthy, S.D., Hindell, M.A., and McKenzie, J. 2002b. Interspecific differences in male vocalisations of three sympatric fur seals (Arctocephalus spp.). Journal of Zoology London 258, 49-56. Peterson, R.S. and Bartholomew, G.A. 1969. Airborne vocal communication in the California sea lion, Zalophus californianus. Animal Behaviour 17, 17- 24. Phillips, A. V. and Stirling, I. 2001. Vocal repertoire of South American fur seals, Arctocephalus Australis: structure, function, and context. Canadian Journal of Zoology 79, 420-437. Phillips, A. V. and Stirling, I. 2000. Vocal individuality in mother and pup South American fur seals, Arctocephalus australis. Marine Mammal Science 16, 592-616. Poole, T.B. 1985. Social behaviour in Mammals. Chapman and Hall, New York, U.S.A. Richards, D. G., and R. H. Wiley. 1980. Reverberations and amplitude fluctuations in the propagation of sound in a forest: implications for animal communication. American Naturalist 115, 381-399. Riedman, M. 1990. The Pinnipeds: seals, sea lions, and walruses. University of California Press, Ltd., Oxford, England. Robisson, P., Aubin, T. and Bremond, J-C. 1993. Individuality in the voice of the Emperor Penguin Aptenodytes forsteri: Adaptation to a noisy environment. Ethology 94, 279-290. Rogers, T.L. 2003. Factors influencing the acoustic behaviour of male phocid seals. Aquatic mammals 29, 247-260. Rogers, T.L. 1996. Acoustic behaviour of the leopard seal, Hydrurga leptonyx: physical characteristics and functional significance. PhD Thesis. University of Sydney. 141 Roux, J.-P. 1986). Sociobiologie de l'Otarie à fourrure d'Amsterdam, Arctocephalus tropicalis. Thèse de Doctorat: Université de Montpellier. Roux, J-P. and Jouventin, P. 1987. Behavioural cues to individual recognition in the Subantarctic fur seal, Arctocephalus tropicalis. In: Status, biology, and ecology of fur seals; Proceedings of an international symposium and workshop Cambridge, England, 23-27 April 1984. (J.P. Croxall, and R.L. Gentry eds.). NOAA Tech. Rep. NMFS 51. Cambridge, England. p.95-103. Sanvito, S. and Galimberti, F. 2000. Bioacoustics of southern elephant seals. I. Acoustic structure of male aggressive vocalisations. Bioacoustics 10, 259- 285. Scherrer, J.A. and Wilkinson, G.S. 1993. Evening bat isolation calls provide evidence for heritable signatures. Animal Behaviour 46, 847-860. Schusterman, R.J. 1977. Temporal patterning in sea lion barking (Zalophus californianus). Behavioral biology 20, 404-408. Schusterman, R.J., Hanggi, E.B. and Gisiner, R. 1992. Acoustic signalling in mother-pup reunions, inter-species bonding, and affiliation by kinship in Californian sea lions (Zalophus californianus). In: Marine Mammal Sensory Systems. (Thomas, J.A., Kastelein, R.A. and Supin, A.Y. eds). Plenum Press, New York. p. 533-551. Schusterman, R.J., Reichmuth, C., and Kastak, D. 2000. How animals classify friends and foes. Current directions in psychological science 9, 1-6. Serrano, A. and Terhune, J.M. 2001. Within-call repetition may be an anti- masking strategy in underwater calls of harp seals (Pagophilus groenlandicus). Canadian Journal of Zoology 79, 1410-1413. Shaughnessy, P.D. 1999. The Action Plan for Australian seals. Environment Australia: Canberra, Australia. Shaughnessy, P.D., and Warneke, R.M. 1987. Australian fur seal, Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus. In: Status, biology, and ecology of fur seals; Proceedings of an international symposium and workshop Cambridge, England, 23-27 April 1984. (J.P. Croxal and R.L. Gentry eds). NOAA Tech. Rep. NMFS 51. Cambridge, England. p. 73-77. Slater, P.J.B. 1974. The temporal pattern of feeding in the zebra finch. Animal Behaviour 22, 506-515. 142 Slater, P.J.B. and Lester, N.P. 1982. Minimising errors in splitting behaviour into bouts. Behaviour 79, 153-161. Snowdon, C.T. and Elowson, A.M. 1992. Ontogeny of primate vocal communication. In: Topics in Primatology. Volume 1. Human Origins. (T. Neshida, W.C., McGrew, P. Marler, M. Pickford and F.B.M., de Waal. eds.). University of Tokyo Press. p. 279-289. Sokal, R.R. and Rohlf, F.J. 1985. Biometry: the principles and practice of statistics in biological research. W.H. Freeman and Company New York, United States of America. Stirling, I. 1975. Adoptive suckling in pinnipeds. Journal of the Australian Mammal Society 1,389-391. Stirling, I. and Warneke, R.M. 1971. Implications of a comparison of the airborne vocalisations and some aspects of the behaviour of the two Australian fur seals. Artcocephalus spp, on the evolution and present taxonomy of the genus. Australian Journal of Zoology 19, 227-241. Stirling, I., Calvert, W., and Spencer, C. 1987. Evidence of stereotyped underwater vocalisations of male Atlantic walruses (Odobenus rosmarus rosmarus). Canadian Journal of Zoology 65, 2311-2321. Stoddard, P.K., Beecher, M.D., Horning, C.L. and Willis, M.S. 1990. Strong neighbour-stranger discrimination in song sparrows. The Condor 92, 1051- 1056. Temeles, E.J. 1994. The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when are they ‘dear enemies’? Animal Behaviour 47, 339-350. Temeles, E.J. 1990. Northern harriers on feeding territories respond more aggressively to neighbours than to floaters. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 26, 57-63. Tooze, Z.J., Harrington, F.H. and Fentress, J.C. 1990. Individually distinct vocalisations in timber wolves, Canis lupus. Animal Behaviour 40, 723- 730. Trillmich, F. 1981. Mutual mother-pup recognition in Galapagos fur seals and sea lions: cues used and functional significance. Behaviour 78, 21-42. 143 Tripovich, J.S. 1999. Acoustic behaviour of Australian fur seals, Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus. Graduate Diploma Thesis. University of New South Wales. Tripovich, J.S., Rogers, T.L., Canfield, R. and Arnould, J.P.Y. 2006. Individual variation in the pup attraction call produced by female Australian fur seals during early lactation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 120, 502-509. Tripovich, J.S., Rogers, T.L. and Arnould, J.P.Y. 2005. Species-specific characteristics and individual variation of the Bark Call produced by male Australian fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus). Bioacoustics 15, 79- 96. Trivers, R.L. 1974. Parent-offspring conflict. American Zoologist 14, 249-264. Van Opzeeland, I.C. and Van Parijs, S.M. 2004. Individuality in harp seal, Phoca groenlandica, pup vocalisations. Animal Behavour 68, 1115-1123. Waas, J.R., Colgan, P.W., and Boag, P.T. 2005. Playback of colony sound alters the breeding schedule and clutch size in zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) colonies. Proceedings of Biological Science 272, 383-383. Warneke, R. 1979. Australian fur seals. In: Mammals of the seas. Volume II. Pinniped Species Summaries and Report on Sirenians. FAO Fisheries Series No.5, Volume II. p. 41-44. Warneke, R.M. and Shaughnessy, P.D. 1985. Arctocephalus pusillus, the South African and Australian Fur Seal: taxonomy, biogeography and life history. In: Studies of Sea Mammals in South Latitudes. (J.K. Ling and M.M. Bryden eds.). South Australian Museum: Adelaide. p. 53-77. Warneke, R.M. 1982. The distribution and abundance of seals in the Australasian region, with summaries of biology and current research. In: Mammals in the seas, Volume IV. Small cetaceans, seals, sirenians and otters. FAO Fisheries Series No. 5., Food and Agriculture Organization, Rome, Italy. p. 431-475 Wickens, P. and York, A.E. 1997. Comparative population dynamics of fur seals. Marine Mammal Science 13, 241-292. Wiley, R. H., and D. G. Richards. 1982. Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: sound propagation and signal detection. In 144 Acoustic Communication in Birds, Volume 1. (D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller eds.). Academic Press, New York. p. 131-181. Wiley, R.H. and Richards, D.G. 1978. Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: implication for the evolution of animal vocalisations. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 3, 69-94. Wynen, L. P., Goldsworthy, S. D., Insley, S. J., Adams, M., Bickham, J. W., Francis, J., Gallo, J. P., Hoelzel, A. R., Majluf, P., White, R. W. G. and Slade,R. 2001. Phylogenetic relationships within the eared seals (Otariidae: Carnivora): implications for the historical biogeography of the family. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 21, 270 -284. Zimmerman, E. and Lerch, C. 1993. The complex acoustic design of an advertisement call in male mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus, Prosimii, Primates) and sources of its variation. Ethology 93, 211-224.

Các file đính kèm theo tài liệu này:

  • pdf02whole.pdf
  • pdf01front.pdf
Tài liệu liên quan